SCHOLARFOX
  • Blog
  • FOCUS
  • EDITOR
Clever as a Fox, 
Write for the World 

More than an Easter Bunny: 5 Famous Rabbits From Around the World

3/31/2019

 
PictureImage by strengthinnumbers via Pixabay.com
More than an Easter Bunny: 5 Famous Rabbits From Around the World
By Heath Shive


1)  Why do we have an Easter Bunny?  

The use of an Easter bunny was formerly credited to the Saxon goddess of spring, Eostre.  But the only surviving mention of this goddess is by the medieval historian the Venerable Bede - who doesn't associate a rabbit with her at all! 

Ancient and medieval cultures around the world associated rabbits with springtime.  Rabbits begin their mating season as early as February and continue to September.  The rabbits would chase each other in courtship which - when combined with their large litter sizes - made rabbits a symbol of the growing fertility of the spring season. 

The pagan symbolism of the rabbit was vouchsafed by the early Christian church. 

But, the use of a rabbit who hides colored eggs for Easter is generally credited to the German Lutherans.  Early Protestants - unlike old-school Catholics - didn't abstain from eating eggs during Lent. 

Eggs were colored as apropos for the season.  But while many ancient cultures decorated their eggs, it was the Germans who created the Osterhase, or what we call the Easter Bunny, a rabbit who brings colored eggs for children.  The tradition was brought to America by German immigrants. 

Sounds crazy?  But rabbits are often thought of as crazy - which is why the British have an expression...

2) ...Mad as a March hare.  Rabbits have an infamous fecundity - producing large litters after relatively short gestation periods.  This hyperactive fertility can cause males to act strangely - running in circles and hopping vertically.  Males can get too aggressive in the their courtship - which is why females often fight to protect themselves.  A lawn full of fighting  fertile rabbits?  Rabbits would look crazy, hence "mad as a March hare." 

The most famous March Hare is the character in Alice's Adventures in Wonderland. 

Many cultures didn't just think a rabbit was "mad," but instead was...

3) ...a masterful trickster.  

The rabbit trickster is very common in Northern Native cultures, but he has a different name wherever you go.  The rabbit trickster was called Glosscap by Eastern Native nations and Wisakedjak by the Cree farther west.  

Nanabozho was a trickster spirit, a primordial deity born at the beginning of time.  He was also a lying, stealing, and manipulative con-man too.  Nanabozho could take many forms, but was a rabbit most frequently.
​
But the trickster rabbit was also famous in western and southern African cultures!  The people of Senegal called him Leuk. 

4) But we know him better as Br'er Rabbit, or Brother Rabbit.  

Br'er Rabbit is the star of many of the Uncle Remus stories of the Old South.  He outwits kings and peasants and every predator in the woods with his wits alone.  Br'er Rabbit - like all tricksters - is morally ambiguous.  He can be larcenous or heroic.  But he always proves that even the physically small and weak can win - provided they have the wits. 

Br'er Rabbit has fallen into disfavor in the current political climate, but...

5)...the trickster rabbit survives today as Bugs Bunny!  

Bugs Bunny is the premier character of the Warner Brothers cartoon classics.  Bugs is flippant, insouciant, and confident.  Bugs is shown to be able to outsmart any of his antagonists - all of whom are trying to do Bugs harm.  But while talking with a Brooklyn accent, and starting his repartee with a "What's up, doc?", Bugs always proves his nonchalant superiority and becomes the very model of cartoon "cool." 

According to Guinness World Records, Bugs Bunny is the 9th most portrayed film personality in the world!  Bugs even has his own star on the Hollywood Walk of Fame. 

LIKE SCHOLARFOX ON FACEBOOK!      

Devil In A Red Dress: How Color Affects Men, Women, and Our Sex Drives

3/24/2019

 
PicturePicture by rigodiaz from Pixabay.com
Devil in a Red Dress: How Color Affects Men, Women, and Our Sex Drives 
By Heath Shive

Did you know that men tip waitresses more when the women wear red?

Psychologist Nicolas Gueguen published a paper on this little color insight.

But it doesn’t stop there.  

The story goes that matadors use red capes because the color provokes the bulls, and baboons flash a red butt to signal status. 

Does red bring out the animal…in you?

This is called embodied cognition, and psychologist Thalma Lobel has written an entire book about it called Sensation: The New Science of Physical Intelligence – which is where I found following studies. 

To the science!

Are Men Suckers for Red?

Researchers Andrew Elliot and Daniela Niesta published a study in 2008.  They showed 2 groups of men a black-and-white photo of a young woman for about 5 seconds.  Both groups of men saw the same woman - but half of the men saw on the picture with a red background and the other half saw the picture on a white background.

The men were then asked to rate the woman’s attractiveness on a scale of 1 to 9. 

The men who saw the woman on a red background rated her as more attractive than the men who saw the white background.

Typical oversexed men, right?

But...

Are Women Suckers for Red Too?

In 2010, researchers (again including Elliot and Niesta) performed a similar experiment on women.  Women were shown the picture of a man.  But the women either saw the picture with a red, white or gray background. 

As expected, women who saw the picture of the man with a red background rated him as more attractive and more desirable than the women who saw the other backgrounds. 


Red has a measurable effect on humans. 

But don’t get carried away, guys.  Just because an effect is measurable does not mean it is of primary significance.  
 
Though women seem to succeed with wearing red dresses, there’s a reason men don’t wear red suits, looking like cartoonish villains. 

But observe the color of a politician’s tie, a rich man’s car, or even a stop sign.

Red gets our attention.

Conclusion

Psychology frequently tells us that our instincts and experiences (our insides) affect our actions in the outside world.

But studies can also show that the outside world (its colors, smells, sounds, and textures) affect our inner selves and thinking too. 

Whether we admit it or not, we are all are sensual creatures.

LIKE SCHOLARFOX ON FACEBOOK!
Sources:
A. J. Elliot and D. Niesta (2008).  Romantic red: Red enhances men’s attraction to women.  Journal Personality and Social Psychology, 95 (5), 1150-1164.

A.J. Elliot, D. Niesta Kayser, T. Greitenmeyer, S. Lichtensfeld, R. H. Gramzow, M. A. Maier, and H. Liu (2010). Red, rank, and romance in women viewing men. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139 (3), 399-417.

Lobel, Thalma.  Sensation: The New Science of Physical Intelligence.  Atria Books, 2014. 

N. Gueguen and C Jacob (2012). Clothing color and tipping: Gentlemen patrons give more tips to waitresses with red clothes. Journal of Hospitality and Tourism Research, April 18, 2012.


The First Cherry Blossoms of Washington D.C...Were Burned to Protect America from Foreigners?

3/17/2019

 
PictureImage by Public Domain Images by Pixabay.com
The First Cherry Blossoms of Washington D.C...Were Burned to Protect America from Foreign Invasion?
By Heath Shive

The famous cherry trees of Washington, D.C., will begin blooming on March 20, peaking on April 4th.  

The trees are beautiful, iconic, and worthy of an American's bucket list.

And the first batch was destroyed to protect America from foreign invaders. 

Why?

To the history!

The Food Explorer David Fairchild

In the later 1800s, the U.S. adopted a largely unregulated gold standard which – when combined with mass production – created what economists called “the Long Depression.”

Between 1870 and 1895, the price for a bushel of corn dropped from 43 cents to 30 cents.  A bushel of wheat worth $1.06 in 1870 had lost almost 40 percent of its value by 1900.  
 
Realizing that American farmers needed more variety than just corn and wheat, the U.S. Department of Agriculture sent out "food explorer" David Fairchild to gather seeds and plantings from around the world. 

Fairchild is credited with introducing nectarines, avocadoes, mangoes, soybeans, and kale to the U.S.  

But Fairchild’s wife Marian loved Japanese flowering cherry trees, and when the Fairchilds finally bought their first home outside D.C. in 1906, Marian ordered over 100 flowering cherry trees. 

The Fairchild’s lawn become famous in the capital.

Flower Power

Then-president William Howard Taft had also been to Japan and had loved their flowering cherry trees. But here’s the thing: mainstream America was xenophobic just then.  There had been a general ban on all Asian immigrants – especially Chinese – for decades.  By 1907, Japanese and Korean immigrants in California were forced into segregated neighborhoods. 

And it wasn’t good to make Japan mad.  Japan was a rising world power.  From 1895 to 1905, Japan had successfully won wars against China and Russia. 

Planting beautiful Japanese flowering cherry trees in America's capital carried political importance.   

So 300 flowering cherry trees were shipped from Tokyo to Washington, D.C.

There was just one problem.  Or rather, thousands of tiny problems.
 
The Fruit Man vs. the Bug Man

Charles Marlatt was the USDA’s chief entomologist.  When California’s crops were being destroyed by the San Jose scale (an invasive insect), it was Marlatt who had saved the day by bringing back the scale’s natural predator - ladybugs. 

Yes, Charles Marlatt was the man who introduced ladybugs to the United States!  But during the trip to Asia, Marlatt's wife contracted disease and died.  

Foreign crops were Fairchild's fame, but Marlatt's pain. 

Marlatt inspected the imported cherry trees and found them rife with pestilence: root gall, 2 kinds of scale bugs, a new species of borer, and 6 more dangerous insect species. 

Taft had no choice but to burn the entire lot of trees, which newspapers made into a public spectacle.   

Taft and the diplomatic corps were afraid of Japan’s reaction, but Japan expressed only shame – shame that they had given so inferior a gift. 

Conclusion

New trees were sent, and these trees passed even Marlatt’s scrutiny.

And on March 27, 1912, First Lady Helen Taft planted the first tree and the Japanese ambassador’s wife planted the second tree. 

A century later, the trees still bloom, and national news networks use the blossoming as a kind of harbinger of spring.

In fact, Americans love the trees so much, that no one cares that these trees don’t make cherries.

LIKE SCHOLARFOX ON FACEBOOK!

Sources:
Stone, Daniel.  The Food Explorer: The True Adventures of the Globe-Trotting Botanist Who Transformed What America Eats. Dutton, 2018.

Bad Choices, Bad Turns, Bad Relationships: The Psychology of Stubborn

3/3/2019

 
PictureImage by MoteOo, accessed via Pixabay.com
Bad Choices, Bad Turns, Bad Relationships: The Psychology of Stubborn
By Heath Shive
​
Obstinate people think that it is a virtue to be stubborn.   

Why is it hard for us to change our minds, our jobs, our relationships, or our lives?

There is no inherent virtue in inflexibility.  After all, the chair that I sit on is unchanging – but my chair is neither faithful nor wise. 

But, scientifically, all humans might be programmed to be a little stubborn. 

Why?

To the science!

My Mug, My Chocolate, My Choice?

In a 1989 paper, economist Jack Knetsch asked one group of students to choose between a coffee mug and a chocolate bar.

Of these students, 44% wanted the chocolate bar, 56% wanted mug. 

Knetsch gave a second group only coffee mugs – but they could exchange for chocolate later, if they wanted. 

Knetsch gave a third group nothing but chocolate bars – but they could exchange for mugs later, if they wanted. 

One would expect that 44% of the students would have traded mugs for chocolate, and that 56% would have traded chocolate for mugs, right? 

No!  Only about 10% of the students wanted to exchange!

Knetsch’s experiment is used to demonstrate the endowment effect – that a person ascribes more value to something when he owns it. 

But psychologist Richard Thaler interpreted the experiment differently in his book Nudge. 

Thaler thought the experiment demonstrated “choice inertia” – that we do not like to change our minds.

We have an instinct to be stubborn.

Stubborn In The Face of Facts

Social psychologists M. Deutsch and H. Gerard performed a study on 3 different groups of college students.  The students had to estimate the lengths of some lines.

One group guessed mentally without revealing their estimate.  Another group had to write down their estimates, sign the paper, and hand these papers to the experimenter. 

All students could change their mind as new evidence was introduced.

The students that only mentally guessed were the most likely to change their minds.  But the students that wrote, singed, announced their guess were the least likely to change their decision.

The more effort we put into a decision, the more stubborn we become.

Betting On the Wrong Horse

Back in the 1960s, psychologists R. E. Knox and J. A. Inkster performed a study at a horse track.  Knox and Inkster discovered that gamblers were more confident about their bet after they made their gamble.

There was no change in facts.  The horses, the track, and the weather were all the same.  But 30 seconds after they made their bet, they were more confident of winning.

Humans can delude themselves about the facts, if it reinforces their earlier decision.

To be stubborn is to be deluded.

Conclusion

Why are we pre-disposed to stick to our choices?

There is something called prospect theory, which – and this is my translation – means that we make choices based on imagined results, rather than factual probability. 

Nobody gains skill, romance, or achievement after only one attempt.

We have to try and try again – imagining that someday we will win or that we will be proven right.  This is perseverance - and it might be a human instinct.

So what is the difference between stubborn and perseverance?

We are stubborn when we pervert our innate optimism to serve our egos, in spite of the facts and results.

LIKE SCHOLARFOX ON FACEBOOK!

Sources:
Deutsch, M., and H. B. Gerard. “A Study of Normative and Informational Social Influences upon Individual Judgment.” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51 (1955): 629-36.

Knetsch, Jack L., "The Endowment Effect and Evidence of Nonreversible Indifference Curves," American Economic Review, 1989, 79, 1277-1284.

Knox, R. E., and J. A. Inkster.  “Postdecisional Dissonance at Post Time.”  Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 8(1968): 319-323. 

Thaler, Ricard. Nudge: Improving Decisions About Health, Wealth, and Happiness. Yale University Press, 2008.     


    Author

    Hello!  My name is Heath Shive, content manager at ScholarFox.  I'll be the author of most of the blog posts.   I'm a former geologist and currently a freelance writer.  The world is complex and seemingly crazy.  Good!  Because when you love to learn, you'll never be bored.

    Archives

    July 2019
    June 2019
    May 2019
    April 2019
    March 2019
    February 2019
    January 2019
    December 2018
    November 2018
    October 2018
    September 2018
    August 2018
    July 2018
    June 2018
    May 2018
    April 2018
    March 2018
    February 2018
    January 2018
    December 2017
    November 2017
    October 2017
    September 2017
    August 2017
    July 2017
    June 2017
    May 2017
    April 2017
    March 2017

    Categories

    All
    Earth Science/Fossils
    Gems/Geology
    History/Culture
    Math
    Modern Living
    PersonalFinance
    Psych/Soc
    Romance
    The Universe/Physics
    Weddings

    RSS Feed

Proudly powered by Weebly
  • Blog
  • FOCUS
  • EDITOR